Thursday, May 16, 2019

Bystander Intervention

Bystander Intervention 1-Social psychological science Eye Skip to contentHomeAb give awayDisclaimerFeatured Journals new(a)s Editors Bystanders just standing by. When do battalion cooperate and when do they non? stick on on March 13, 2011 by ezaiser 1 Comment By Erica Zaiser Understanding when and why population interject to befriend separate(a)s, or when they dont, is at the heart of cortele visit dial psychology. All school-age childs of psychology lease the famous field of study of mass Genovese, whose screams while beingness attacked shited to elicit admirer oneself from the nearly 40 bystanders. close to research on bystander intervention has tack that the size of the group greatly imp puzzle proscribeds the same(p)lihood of intervention.Too cock-a-hoop of a group and everybody shifts obligation assuming that nearone else get out sustain scarcely the more than race the minute in all probability that any single set out behind help. It master ms hard to imagine that populate would non help when soul is in trouble, wounded, or in danger, yet it happens tout ensemble the time. Recently I myself stumblight-emitting diode upon a jibe of bystander non-intervention which I stupefy since struggled to understand. The an opposite(prenominal) day while walking home I came upon a earthly misgiving running up and down the channel with no shoes or coat holding a phone out shouting at the citizenry on the street and stopping cars banging on the windows.I took a second to survey the scene and it was fix this man was trying to get roughlything from those somewhat him. However nobody was closureing him and none of the cars thus far rolled down their windows to listen. I heard his head phrases tawdry and clear, albeit in broken English, How to predict an ambulance? Still nobody was saying anything. I shouted to him that he indispens fit to mobilize 999 and he came all over profusely grateful for my help and I hel ped him set out his need conjure and assisted him and his family until paramedics could arrive. His m different had f wholeen unconscious in their flat and he had run into the street esperate to hunch forward how to c either requisite services in this country. I learned that he and all his family was from eastern europium and they knew very myopic English. He also told me that he had been trying to get the number for quite some time scarce nobody had been instinctive to help. Having read work on bystander behaviour I shouldnt boast been that surprised that nobody helped but the built in bed just didnt fit the common tactual sensation that with greater numbers passel are less likely to help. Most of the famous adventures involving non- circumstances behaviour has been inwardly large assemblys. on that point were maybe 7 or 10 deal on the street when I arrived. Most were just standing and watching. I dont demand a great answer for why people didnt help, maybe they c ouldnt understand his question but it seemed quite clear to me. Maybe they feared that it was some type of scam.. but certainly it loafert hurt to ordain soul a phone number. take down more frustrating than not understanding the lack of help was the sneak suspicion that had he been British, white, or at to the lowest degree a native English speaker, maybe mortal would commence helped. Research by Levine and colleagues suggests that in that respect dexterity be an element of truth to that.In a study of non-intervention, their research suggests that bystanders are more than more likely to help people when they feel that the soul seeking assistance is part of their ingroup. This effect holds true compensate when controlling for the severity of the situation and the ruttish arousal felt by bystanders. In some former(a)wise words, no matter how bad the situation or how badly the bystanders felt, they were still less likely to help when the dupe was an outgroup section. - This all makes sense from a social psychological perspective and logical arguments up with other research.People tend to be pay defend better to people in their own group in general. and seeing it play out was still a shortsighted depressing. 2-Masculinity inhibits helping in emergencies disposition does predict the bystander effect. By Tice, Dianne M. Baumeister, Roy F. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol 49(2), Aug 1985, 420-428. Abstract tried and true 4 competing hypotheses (masculinity as enhancer, femininity as enhancer, interactive, masculinity as inhibitor) regarding the potential effects of dispositional sex-role orientation on bystander intervention in emergencies. 0 undergraduates, classified on the Bem Sex-Role Inventory, participated in a simulated group treatment via headphones. One member of the group apparently had a choking fit and called for help. Highly masculine Ss were less likely to take action to help the victim than were other Ss. Femin inity and actual gender had no effect on likelihood of helping. Results are interpreted according to past research evidence that passing masculine Ss fear potential embarrassment and loss of poise, so they may be reluctant to intervene in emergencies. (27 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) ___________________________________________________________ _____________________- 3-HELP NOW CONSENT Confidential Reporting HOME How to HELP What is ? sum total Use and Sexual Assault Parents Faculty Staff Community Commitment Education Opportunities F A Qs Police Services and Legal Issues Bystanders Can jockstrap A bystander is individual in a crowd who sees a potentially dangerous situation and makes a choice to assist or not to assist. A bystander can protect the values of safety, trust, and honor that are central to our community. The Good Samaritan Less common than you might destine.In 1968 researchers Darley & Latane conducted an experiment in which a s tudent pretended to hand a seizure and the experimenters record how often others stopped to help. When totally one bystander was watching the scene, the student was helped 85% of the time. However, if in that respect were five bystanders, the student was nevertheless helped 31% of the time. Does this make sense? Shouldnt having more people afford increase the chances that individual will get help? Amazingly, this is not the case. We all take cues from those around us approximately how to act in different situations.In emergency situations, many things prohibit bystanders from intervening If no one else is acting, it is hard to go against the crowd. People may feel that they are run a risking embarrassment. (What if Im rail at and they dont need help? ) They may think there is someone else in the group who is more qualified to help. They may think that the situation does not call for help since no one else is doing anything. With each individual taking cues from people aro und them, a common reply is that no action is taken. What can we do close to this problem?As members of the WSU community we all have a responsibility to help each other. Avoid being a bystander Intervene disregardless of what others are doing and dont be worried about being wrong it is better to be wrong than to have done nothing at all. 1. I am a bystander. What can I do? Be on the verbalism-out for potentially dangerous situations. Learn how to recognize indications of potentially dangerous situations. Here are some examples of red flag behaviors related to sexual assault Inappropriate touching Suggestive remarks examen boundaries do by set boundaries Inappropriate intimacy Attempts to isolate someone Pressuring someone to drink Violent behaviors Targeting someone who is visibly imp haloed 2. If I were in this situation, would I unavoidableness someone to help me? If a situation makes us uncomfor slacken, we may try to dismiss it as not being a problem. You may tell yours elf that the other soul will be fine, that he or she is not as intoxicated as you think, or that the person is able to defend him/herself. This is not a solution The person may need your help more than you think When in doubt, TRUST YOUR GUT. Instincts are there for a reason.When a situation makes us feel uncomfortable, it is a generally a good indicator that something is not right. It is better to be wrong about the situation than do nothing. Many people feel reluctant to intervene in a situation because they are afraid of making a scene or feel as though a person would ask for help if it were call for. 3. You have the responsibility to intervene. You may be thinking No one else is helping it must not be a problem People who are dismal dont think this is a problem, maybe Im wrong? Jims sincerely prudent and hes not intervening why should I?Many people do not intervene in a potentially dangerous situation because they are looking to others for cues on how to act or they believe someone else will intervene. precisely IT IS YOUR responsibility to act as a Cougar, as a friend to all other students, and as a member of WSUs community of trust and safety. 4. You have the skills to act Learn effective intervention techniques fall out out for other members of the WSU community Come up with a plan beforehand Talk to your friends about how they would privation you to intervene if they are in an uncomfortable situation. Choose the intervention strategy that is best for the situation. push a breath and make your move References Berkowitz, A. Understanding the role of bystander behavior. US Department of Educations 20th one-year National Meeting on Alcohol and Other Drug Abuse and Violence Pr takingsion in Higher Education, Arlington, VA Darley, J. M. , Latane, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 377-383. Cialdini, R. B. (2001) Influence Science and Practice. Needham Hei ghts, MA Allyn Bacon -Counseling Services, PO Box 641065, Washington State University, Pullman WA 99164- 4- bran-new York News Views Interactive Reporting from CUNY grade School of Journalism office Skip to contentHomeAboutWhat Would You Do? NYC Robbery Bystanders Fail to Help Posted on October 8, 2010 by Brendaliss Gonzalez Courtesy NYPD CompStat Unit You think youre the only one, and then you remember, you live in New York- youre never the only one. According to New York City Police Department depicts, by folk this year, 66,691 people had been victims of robbery, including assault, burglary and grand larceny.Its amazing how many people can tell you their story of being mugged in the city, even more surprising are the stories that occurred in broad daylight, with witnesses who seemed to have pulled a disappearance act during the occurrence. Two weeks ago, a pair of robberies at ATMs in Columbus order and West 23rd occurred in broad daylight, shocking each of the victims who believed they were playing it safe by leaving out at early hours. The report fails to mention anyone around them stopping to help. A pregnant cleaning lady was also robbed and attacked in Gramercy Park when coming home from a doctors ap stagecoachment- any witnesses?Who knows? And lets not forget the story in April when a homeless man lay unused for hours aft(prenominal) being knifed to death in a heroic attempt to keep on a womanhood being robbed- witnesses and passerbys caught on camera walking past the dead body without even calling for help. The excuse? Most assumed another already called the police. You would think that with so many people having experienced being mugged, most would readily lend a hand or just dial 911 when seeing someone else be mugged. Yet most of the time, no one even flinches.In a busy, dog-eat-dog city like New York, the attitude seems to be more of each man for himself. - So, this poses a question that will require you to look deep down and really be honest. Would you stop to help someone being robbed or assaulted? Or would you carry them to fight their own fight? Besides, you dont want to have to relive that kind of experience, putting yourself in danger that would just be stupid, right? 5-The Bystander put Carol Hensell Program Manager ADHS SVPEP Phoenix, AZ October 2009If you work in the field of violence prevention, you are belike familiar with the story of Kitty Genovese. In New York, 1964 Kitty Genovese was murdered on the street while 38 witnesses watched from their flatcars and failed to intervene. Her story has become influential to the field of social psychology and has promoted the development of suppositions around the psychology of helping or bystander effect (Latane Darley, 1970). The bystander effect is described as the idea that individuals are more likely to help when altogether than when in the company of others (Latane Darley, 1970).There is a large amount of books examining helping behaviors and trying to understand under what conditions do people decide to help others and models of the bystander effect have developed over time. The literature includes stu lives that exa exploit individual and situational circumstanceors that promote or hinder pro-social bystander intervention (Banyard, Moynihan, Plante, 2007). Factors that have been entrap to affect helping behavior are group size, which accounts for the airing of responsibility or the idea that someone else will intervene. Perceptions and reactions to situations are negatively affected by the forepart of other people.These perceptions can be either real or imagined. Other studies have found that if a group is cohesive and confabulation occurs, a consensus to help develops and they are more likely to intervene (Banyard, Moynihan, Plante, 2007). Living in a uncouth environment may increase the likelihood of someone intervening (Banyard, Moynihan, Plante, 2007). Inter face-to-face factors that affect if a person inte rvenes includes mood, individual perceptions of the event, mood, nature of relationship to the person in need of help, and perceptions that will be able to truly help the person (Banyard, Moynihan, Plante, 2007).There appears to be ambiguity around intervening in several situations, curiously those that are violent. Norms about what is appropriate and inappropriate behavior in particular social contexts are found in most aspects of individuals daily lives and they also exist in the area of helping behaviors (Hart Miethe, 2008). Understanding these norms can facilitate a greater understanding of bystander behaviors and contribute to creation effective programs for change magnitude bystander certifiedness and behaviors in the area of sexual violence prevention.Exploring the bystander effect is important because bystander actions and reactions may affect two the risks of violence and consequences of violence for a victim. A witness or bystander may deter a plague from occurring or their intervention may help a victim if a violent attack is in progress (Hart Miethe, 2008). Many people believe that violent curses occur in secluded throw ins out of the site of others. However, many crimes are committed in the presence of a social audience (Hart Miethe, 2008).According to a National Crime Victimization Survey (NCVS) completed in the 1990s, bystanders were present in approximately 70% of assaults, 52% of robberies, and 29% of rapes and sexual assault (Planty, 2002 as cited in Hart Miethe, 2008). - When set about with a potentially dangerous situation, bystanders have choices. They can choose to do nothing, provide in lead support (calling police or others to help), or directly intervene. 6 - RemNot So Innocent Bystanders By Sara on March 11, 2010 338 PM 1 Comment 0 TrackBacks Should bystanders of crime be convicted? There is almost eer something that a bystander can do to help stop crime against another human. If the criminal is waving a weapon syst em around, it is understandable that not many bystanders would footmark up to the plate. However, there have been many cases lately that have shown how little bystanders do to help a person in need, when they are fully able to. many of these bystanders really JOIN the perpetrator.The links I have posted here show video of a woman being beaten in a subway, with subway officers there. The officers say that it is not their job to tonus in, and they called for reinforcement. Whoever said that maltreatping in is not permitting was obviously not there, and did not see how important it is that they DO step in. The second video is a news report of a high school girl who was hoop raped outside of her homecoming dance. People watched and jeered, and some who had just been walking by joined in to rape her. Some even recorded the event on their cell-phone cameras.But no one helped these victims. hold up semester I took Social Psychology and learned about the Kitty Genovese case. This wom an was killed outside of her apartment complex as her neighbors watched and listened. They were given ample time to go out and help her or call for police after the killer had left. No one did anything. This is known as the bystander effect, which is sometimes caused by diffusion of responsibility. Bystanders think, Someone else will surely help, someone else has probably already done something, yea, I dont have to do anything. But often no one helpsThis cannot be used as an excuse. These people are almost as guilty as the perpetrator and should be convicted too. TagsBystander,bystander effect,diffusion of responsibilty,Kitty Genovese,Social PsychologyNo TrackBacksember, when people intervene for the good of others, it creates a safer community. 7- Dont notwithstanding Stand There Do Something A community where people intervene for the good of others is a safer community. The Bystander essence Forty years ago, Kitty Genovese was attacked and murdered outside her New York City ap artment building. xxxviii people heard her calls for help s they watched from behind their apartment windows. The attack at long lasted more than half an hour. by and by it was over, someone called the police, who arrived within two transactions. That 1964 incident became a textbook case. Why did so many witnesses fail to act? Phoning the police would involve no risk, and likely would have saved Ms. Genoveses life. Social psychologists Latane and Darley1 suggested reasons such as diffusion of responsibility or failure to recognize the true significance of the incident. They concluded that the more people witness an event, the less likely each individual is to intervene.This became known as the Bystander Effect. When a violent incident or emergency occurs, the Bystander Effect is not a mere academic c one timept. In an unpublicized case last summer, seven young men robbed and knifed the 16-year old nephew of a Canada recourse Council staff member, who happened to be walking thr ough a downtown park in a study Canadian city. No one helped the victim or called the police. If the attackers had been caught, they could have faced criminal charges instead of likely going on to commit more crimes. Someone in the crowd must have had a cell phone.Why didnt anyone at least call the police? Numerous incidents like this happen in communities across Canada. Police estimate that only one out of every 10 swarmings is account. The victims, often teenagers, are left scarred and traumatized for life. Such attacks blend many Canadians to fear their communities are unsafe. This fear only makes matters worse by creating abandoned, dangerous streets. Its not that Canadians dont act when they see an urgent situation. There are countless examples of successful intervention, including people who have risked their life to save a stranger.Nonetheless, police and community safety leaders would like to see more bystander involvement. Simply by reporting an urgent situation, a witne ss can prevent it from becoming more serious. Everyone Can Help How can the power of bystanders be harnessed in the inte ministration of public safety? Several factors can encourage people to help strangers in di dialect. When a victim makes it very clear help is needed, people are more likely to intervene. Dont expect bystanders to figure out youre in trouble. Make sure they know. For example, look directly at someone in the crowd and ask for help.Perceived ability to help and perceived risk also determine whether or not a bystander will help. For example, the ubiquitous cell phone empowers users to call for help from almost anywhere, immediately and with little or no risk. Close to six billion emergency calls are placed from mobile phones in Canada each year about half of all calls to emergency numbers. Every day, thousands of Canadians use mobile phones to call for help when they see a crash, a crime in progress or a life-threatening medical emergency. Police urge witnesses of crimes to be attentive and to call 9-1-1 as quickly as possible.Give a good description of the perpetrators, where they came from and where they go after the incident. In 1993, two-year-old James Bulger was murdered in the UK by two older children. Ironically, 38 witnesses saw the toddler being led apart against his will by two older boys. UK researchers looked at the role of bystanders in the tragedy. Dr. Mark Levine2 found that they did not intervene because they persuasion the trio boys were brothers and considered family a tete-a-tete space. after(prenominal) examining other instances of bystander intervention and non-intervention, Dr.Levine concluded that members of a group take responsibility for the safety of others they see as belong to the same group and that the sense of group membership can be broadened. All Canadians must do their part to ensure we continue to live in a safe and civilized society. When you see someone in trouble just think if you were that perso n, what would you want passers-by to do? 9-1-1 Tips for Mobile Phone Users Calls to 9-1-1 are unloose of charge. Do not preprogram 9-1-1 into your phones speed-dial function. Dial 9-1-1 only when the safety of people or property is at risk (e. . a fire, crime in progress or medical emergency). Provide your 10-digit phone number so the operator can call you back. Give your precise location or the location of the emergency. Describe the emergency all the way. Stay on the line until the operator tells you to hang up. Then, leave your phone saturnine on in case the operator calls back. 1 Latane, Bibb Darley, John M. (1968). Group inhibition of bystander intervention in emergencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10(3), 215-221. 2Levine, Mark (2002). Walk On By?Relational Justice Bulletin (Issue 16, Nov 2002) Safety Canada January 2004 Safety Canada January 2004 - Canadas Silent Tragedy 8- AP PSYCHOLOGY NILAND Chapter 13 Social Psychology paginate 1 of 5 When depart People Help in a Crisis? John M. Darley and Bibb Latane Most of America lives in cities, and it is one of the major tragedies of these times that our cities are in deep trouble. In small towns throughout the country, people still leave their houses unlocked and the keys in their cars when they park.No one living in a rural community would inhalation of stealing from someone else, because everyone knows everyone. Who wants to steal from people he knows? And if you stole a friends car, where could you drive it in a small community that it wouldnt instantly be recognized? When everyone knows everyone, complex social systems are not needed to help alleviate those disasters that strike-the fire and police departments are staffed chiefly by volunteers (who never go on strike), and the welfare department consists of charitable neighbors rather than squads o f social workers.Cities are supposed to be collections of small towns, but in at least one important sense, they are not in a rural community, everyone sees the (often rather crude) machinery of government and feels that it is available to him. In large cities, this machinery is mostly invisible, hidden away in inaccessible Kafkaesque corners. Involvement in local affairs is almost forced on the small-town citizen the apartment dweller in New York withdraws into his own little world not so often because he wants to as because he has no ready means o f participating actively in the life o f his city even if he wants to.And, as John M. Darley and Bibb Latane point out, withdrawal from and lack of concern about ones beastlyow citizens can become a terrible habit. Kitty Genovese is set upon by a maniac as she returns home from work at 3 A. m. Thirty-eight of her neighbors in Kew Gardens come to their windows when she cries out in terror none comes to her assistance even though her stalker takes over half an hour to murder her. No one even so much as calls the police. She dies. Andrew Mormille is stabbed in the sto mach as he rides the A train home to Manhattan.Eleven other riders watch the seventeen-year-old boy as he bleeds to death none comes to his assistance even though his attackers have left the car. He dies. An eighteen-year-old switchboard operator, alone in her office is the Bronx, is raped and beaten. Escaping momentarily, she runs naked and bleeding to the street, emit for help. A crowd of forty passerby gathers and watches as, in broad daylight, the rapist tries to drag her lack upstairs no one interferes. Finally two policemen happen by and arrest her assailant. Eleanor Bradley trips and breaks her leg while obtain on Fifth Avenue.Dazed and in shock, she calls for help, but the hurrying stream of executives and shoppers simply parts and flows past. After forty minutes a taxi driver helps her to a doctor. The shocking thing about these cases is that so many people failed to suffice. If only one or two had ignored the victim, we might be able to understand their inactiveness. But when thirty-eight people, or eleven people, or hundreds of people fail to help, we become disturbed. Actually, this fact that shocks us so much is itself the clue to understanding these cases.Although it seems obvious that the more people who watch a victim in distress, the more likely someone will help, what really happens is exactly the opposite. If each member of a group of bystanders is aware that other people are also present, he will be less likely to notice the emergency, less likely to decide that it is an emergency, and less likely to act even if he thinks there is an emergency. This is a surprising assertion-what we are saying is that the victim may actually be less likely to get help, the more people who watch his distress and are available to help.We shall establish in detail the process through which an individual bystander must go in order to intervene, and we shall present the results of some experiments designed to show the effects of the number of onlookers on the likelihood of intervention. Since we started research on bystander resolutions to emergencies, we have heard many explanations for the lack of intervention. I would assign this to the effect of the megapolis in which we live, which makes closeness very serious and leads to the alienation of the individual from the group, contributed a psychoanalyst. A disaster syndrome, explained a sociologist, that shook the sense of safety and sureness of the individuals involved and caused psychological withdrawal from the event by ignoring it. Apathy, claimed others. Indifference. The gratification of unconscious sadistic impulses. Lack of concern for our fellow men. The Cold Society. All of these analyses of the person who fails to help share one characteristic they set the inert witness apart from the rest of us as a different kind of person.Certainly not one of us who reads about these incidents in horror is apathetic, alienated, or depersonalized. Certainly not AP PSYCHOLOGY NIL AND Chapter 13 Social Psychology Page 2 of 5 one of us enjoys gratifying his sadistic impulses by watching others suffer. These terrifying cases in which people fail to help others certainly have no personal implications for us. That is, we might decide not to ride subways anymore, or that New York isnt even a nice place to visit, or there ought to be a law against apathy, but we need not feel guilty, or reexamine ourselves.Looking more well-nigh at published descriptions of the behavior of witnesses to these incidents, the people involved arrest to look a little less inhuman and a lot more like the rest of us. Although it is unquestionably true that the witnesses in the incidents above did nothing to save the victims, apathy, indifference, and unconcern are not wholly accurate descriptions of their reactions. The thirty-eight witnesses of Kitty Genoveses murder did not merely look at the scene once and then ignore it. They continued to stare out of their windows at what was goi ng on.Caught, fascinated, distressed, unwilling to act but unavailing to turn away, their behavior was neither helpful nor heroic but it was not indifferent or apathetic. Actually, it was like crowd behavior in many other emergency situations. Car accidents, drownings, fires, and attempted suicides all attract substantial numbers of people who watch the drama in helpless fascination without acquire directly involved in the action. Are these people alienated and indifferent? Are the rest of us? Obviously not. Why, then, dont we act?The bystander to an emergency has to make a series of endings about what is happening and what he will do about it. The consequences of these decisions will determine his actions. There are three things he must do if he is to intervene notice that something is happening, interpret that event as an emergency, and decide that he has personal responsibility for intervention. If he fails to notice the event, if he decides that it is not an emergency, or if he concludes that he is not personally responsible for acting, he will leave the victim unhelped. This state of affairs is shown graphically as a decision tree. Only one path through this decision tree leads to intervention all others lead to a failure to help. As we shall show, at each fork of the path in the decision tree, the presence of other bystanders may lead a person down the branch of not helping. Noticing The First Step reflect that an emergency is actually taking place a middle-aged man has a heart attack. He stops short, clutches his chest, and staggers to the nearest building wall, where he slowly slumps to the sidewalk in a sitting position. What is the likelihood that a passerby will come to his assistance?First, the bystander has to notice that something is happening. The external event has to break into his thinking and intrude itself on his conscious mind. He must tear himself away from his private thoughts and pay attention to this unusual event. But Americans co nsider it bad manners to look too closely at other people in public. We are taught to respect the privacy of others, and when among strangers, we do this by end our ears and avoiding staring at others-we are embarrassed if caught doing otherwise. In a crowd, then, each person is less likely to notice the outset sign of a potential emergency than when alone.Experimental evidence corroborates this everyday observation. Darley and Latane asked college students to an query about their reactions to urban living. As the students waited to see the interviewer, either by themselves or with two other students, they gormandize up out a preliminary questionnaire. Solitary students often glanced idly about the get on while pick out their questionnaires those in groups, to avoid seeming rudely inquisitive, kept their eyes on their own papers. As part of the study, we staged an emergency roll of tobacco was released into the delay room through a vent.Twothirds of the subjects who were al one when the smoke appeared noticed it immediately, but only a quarter of the subjects waiting in groups saw it as quickly. Even after the room had completely filled with smoke one subject from a group of three finally looked up and exclaimed, God I must be smoking too much Although at last all the subjects did become aware of the smoke, this study sharpens that the more people present, the slower an individual may be to perceive that an emergency does exist and the more likely he is not to see it at all. formerly an event is noticed, an onlooker must decide whether or not it is truly an emergency.Emergencies are not always clearly labeled as such smoke pouring from a building or into a waiting room may be caused by a fire, or it may merely testify a leak in a steam pipe. Screams -in the street may signal an assault or a family quarrel. A man lying in a doorway may be having a coronary, suffering from diabetic coma, or he may simply be sleeping off a drunken night. And in any un usual situation, Candid Camera may be watching. A person trying to decide whether or not a given situation is an emergency often refers to the reactions of those around him he looks at them to see how he should react himself.If everyone else is calm and indifferent, he will tend to tolerate calm and indifferent if everyone else is reacting strongly, he will become aroused. This tendency is not merely slavish consent ordinarily we derive much valuable information about new situations from how others around us behave. Its a rare traveler who, in picking a roadside restaurant, chooses to stop at one with no cars in the parking lot. AP PSYCHOLOGY NILAND Chapter 13 Social Psychology Page 3 of 5 But occasionally the reactions of others provide false information.The studied nonchalance of patients in a dentists waiting room is a poor indication of the pain awaiting them. In general, it is considered embarrassing to look overly concerned, to seem flustered, to lose your cool down in pub lic. When we are not alone, most of us try to seem less anxious than we really are. In a potentially dangerous situation, then, everyone present will appear more detached than he is in fact. Looking at the apparent impassivity and lack of reaction of the others, each person is led to believe that nothing really is wrong.Meanwhile the danger may be mounting, to the point where a sensation person, uninfluenced by the seeming calm of others, would react. A crowd can thus force inaction on its members by implying, through its passivity and apparent indifference, that an event is not an emergency. Any individual in such a crowd is uncomfortably aware that hell look like a fool if he behaves as though it were-and in these circumstances, until someone acts, no one acts. In the smoke-filled-room study, the smoke trickling from the wall constituted an ambiguous but potentially dangerous situation.How did the presence of other people affect a persons response to the situation? Typically, t hose who were in the waiting room by themselves noticed the smoke at once, gave a slight startle reaction, hesitated, got up and went over to investigate the smoke, hesitated again, and then left the room to find somebody to tell about the smoke. No one showed any signs of panic, but over three-quarters of these people were concerned enough to report the smoke. Others went through an identical experience but in groups of three strangers. Their behavior was radically different.Typically, once someone noticed the smoke, he would look at the other people, see them doing nothing, shrug his shoulders, and then go back to his questionnaire, casting covert glances first at the smoke and then at the others. From these three-person groups, only three out of twenty- quad people reported the smoke. The inhibiting effect of the group was so strong that the other twenty-one were willing to sit in a room filled with smoke rather than make themselves conspicuous by reacting with alarm and concern- this despite the fact that after three or four minutes the tmosphere in the waiting room grew most unpleasant. Even though they coughed, rubbed their eyes, tried to wave the smoke away, and dedicateed the window, they apparently were unable to bring themselves to leave. These dramatic differences between the behavior of people alone and those in a group indicate that the group imposed a definition of the situation upon its members that inhibited action. A leak in the air conditioning, said one person when we asked him what he thought caused the smoke. Must be chemistry labs in the building. go pipes. Truth gas to make us give true answers on the questionnaire, reported the more imaginative. There were many explanations for the smoke, but they all had one thing in common they did not mention the word fire. In defining the situation as a non-emergency, people explained to themselves why the other observers did not leave the room they also removed any reason for action themselves. T he other members of the group acted as non-responsive models for each person-and as an audience for any inappropriate action he might consider. In such a situation it is all too easy to do nothing.The results of this study clearly and strongly support the predictions. But are they general? Would the same effect show up with other emergencies, or is it limited to situations like the smoke study involving danger to the self as well as to others-or to situations in which theres no clearly defined victim? It may be that our college-age male subjects played chicken with one another to see who would lose face by first fleeing the room. It may be that groups were less likely to respond because no particular person was in danger.To see how generalize these results were, Latane and Judith Rodin set up a second experiment, in which the emergency would cause no danger-for the bystander, and in which a specific person was in trouble. Subjects were paid $50 to participate in a survey of game and puzzle preferences conducted at Columbia by the Consumer Testing Bureau (CTB). An attractive young woman, the market-research representative, met them at the door and took them to the testing room. On the way, they passed the CTB office and through its open door they could see filing cabinets and a desk nd bookcases piled high with papers. They entered the adjacent testing room, which contained a table and chairs and a variety of games, where they were given a preliminary background information and game preference questionnaire to fill out. The representative told subjects that she would be working next door in her office for about ten minutes while they completed the questionnaires, and left by enterprisingness the collapsible curtain that divided the two rooms. She do sure the subjects knew that the Curtain was unlocked, easily opened, and a means of entry to her office.The representative stayed in her office, shuffling papers, opening drawers, and making enough noise to remind the subjects of her presence. Four minutes after leaving the testing area, she turned on a high-fidelity stereophonic tape recorder. AP PSYCHOLOGY NILAND Chapter 13 Social Psychology Page 4 of 5 If the subject listened carefully, he heard the representative climb up on a chair to reach for a stack of papers on the bookcase. Even if he were not sense of hearing carefully, he heard a loud crash and a scream as the chair collapsed and she fell to the floor. Oh, my God, my foot . . . I . . . I . . . cant move it. Oh . . . my ankle, the representative moaned. I . . . cant get this . . . thing . . . off me. She cried and moaned for about a minute longer, but the cries gradually got more subdued and controlled. Finally she muttered something about getting outside, knocked over the chair as she pulled herself up, and thumped to the door, closing it behind her as she left. This drama lasted about two minutes. Some people were alone in the waiting room when the accident occurred. Some 70 percent of them offered to help the victim before she left the room.Many came through the curtain to offer their assistance, others simply called out to offer their help. Others faced the emergency in pairs. Only 20 percent of this group eight out of forty offered to help the victim. The other thirty-two remained unresponsive to her cries of distress. Again, the presence of other bystanders inhibited action. And again, the non-interveners seemed to have decided the event was not an emergency. They were unsure what had happened, but whatever it was, it was not too serious. A mild sprain, some said. I didnt want to embarrass her. In a real emergency, they assured us, they would be among the first to help the victim. Perhaps they would be, but in this situation they did not help, because for them the event was not defined as an emergency. Again, solitary people undefended to a potential emergency reacted more frequently than those exposed in groups. We found that the action-inhibitin g effects of other bystanders works in two different situations, one of which involves risking danger to oneself and the other of which involves helping an injured woman.The result seems sufficiently general so that we may assume it operates to inhibit helping in real-life emergencies. Diffused province Even if a person has noticed an event and defined it as an emergency, the fact that he knows that other bystanders also witnessed it may still make him less likely to intervene. Others may inhibit intervention because they make a person feel that his responsibility is diffused and diluted. Each soldier in a loss squad feels less personally responsible for killing a man than he would if he alone pulled the trigger.Likewise, any person in a crowd of onlookers may feel less responsibility for parsimony a life than if he alone witnesses the emergency. If your car breaks down on a busy highway, hundreds of drivers whir by without anyones stopping to help if you are stuck on a nearly d eserted country road, whoever passes you first is apt to stop. The personal responsibility that a passerby feels makes the difference. A driver on a solitary road knows that if he doesnt stop to help, the person will not get help the same individual on the crowded highway feels he personally is no more responsible than any of a hundred other drivers.So even though an event clearly is an emergency, any person in a group who sees an emergency may feel less responsible, simply because any other bystander is every bit responsible for helping. This diffusion of responsibility might have occurred in the famous Kitty Genovese case, in which the observers were walled off from each other in separate apartments. From the silhouettes against windows, all that could be told was that others were also watching. . To test this line of thought, Darley and Latane simulated an emergency in a setting designed to resemble Kitty Genoveses murder. People overheard a victim calling for help.Some knew the y were the only one to hear the victims cries, the rest believed other people were aware of the victims distress. As with the Genovese witnesses, subjects could not see each other or know what others were doing. The kind of direct group inhibition found in the smoke and fallen-woman studies could not operate. For the simulation, we recruited male and female students at New York University to participate in a group discussion. Each student was put in an individual room equipped with a set of headphones and a microphone and told to listen for instructions over the headphones.The instructions inform the participant that the discussion was to consider personal problems of the normal college student in a high-pressure urban university. It was explained that, because participants might feel embarrassed about discussing personal problems publicly, several precautions had been taken to, ensure their anonymity they would not meet the other people face to face, and the experimenter would not listen to the initial discussion but would only ask for their reactions later.Each person was to talk in turn. The first to talk reported that he found it difficult to adjust to New York and his studies. Then, very hesitantly and with obvious embarrassment, he mentioned that he was prone to flyaway seizures, similar to but not really the same as epilepsy. These occurred particularly when he was under the stresses of studying and being graded. Other people then discussed their own problems in turn. The number of other people in the discussion varied.But whatever the perceived size of the group two, three, or six people-only the subject was actually present the others, as well as the instructions and the speeches of the victim-to-be, were present only on a prerecorded tape. When it again was the first persons turn to talk, after a few comments he launched into the following AP PSYCHOLOGY NILAND Chapter 13 Social Psychology Page 5 of 5 performance, getting increasingly louder with increasing speech difficulties I can see a lot of er of er how other peoples problems are similar to mine ecause er er I mean er its er I mean some of the er same er kinds of things that I have and an er Im sure that every everybody has and er er I mean er theyre not er e-easy to handle sometimes and er I er er be upsetting like er er and er I er um I think I I need er if if could er er somebody er er er er er give me give me a little er give me a little help here because er I er Im er h-h-having a a a a a real problem er right now and I er if somebody could help me out it would it would er er s-s-sure be sure be good be . . because er there er er a cause I er uh Ive got a a one of the er seiz-er er things coming on and and and I c-could really er use er some h-help s-so if somebody would er give me a little h-help uh er-er-er-er-er c-could somebody er er help er uh uh uh choking sounds . . . Im gonna die er er Im . . . gonna . . .. die er help er er seizure er er . . . chokes, then quiet. While this was going on, the experimenter waited outside the students door to see how soon he would emerge to move with the emergency.Rather to our surprise, some people sat through the entire fit without helping a disproportionately large percentage of these non-responders were from the largest-size group. Some 85 percent of the people who believed themselves to be alone with the victim came out of their rooms to help, while 62 percent of the people who believed there was one other bystander did so. Of those who believed there were four other bystanders, only 31 percent reported the fit before the tape ended.The responsibility-diluting effect of other people was so strong that single individuals were more than twice as likely to report the emergency as those who thought other people also knew about it. The Moral Dilemma Felt by Those Who Do Not Respond People who failed to report the emergency showed few signs of apathy and indifference thought to characterize unresponsive bystanders. When the experimenter entered the room to end the situation, the subject often asked if the victim was all right. Many of these people showed physical signs of nervousness they often had trembling hands and sweating palms.If anything, they seemed more emotionally aroused than did those who reported the emergency. Their emotional arousal was in sharp contrast to the behavior of the non-responding subjects in the smoke and fallen-woman studies. Those subjects were calm and unconcerned when their experiments were over. Having interpreted the events as non-emergencies, there was no reason for them to be otherwise. It was only the subjects who did not respond in the face of the clear emergency represented by the fit who felt the moral dilemma. Why, then, didnt they respond? It is our stamp that non-intervening subjects had not decided not to respond.Rather, they were still in a state of indecision and conflict concerning whether to respond or not. The emotional behavior o f these non-responding subjects was a sign of their continuing conflict a conflict that other people resolved by responding. The distinction seems an academic one for the victim, since he gets no help in either case, but it is an extremely important one for understanding why bystanders fail to help. The evidence is clear, then, that the presence of other bystanders and the various ways these other bystanders affect our decision processes make a difference in how likely we are to give help in an emergency.The presence of strangers may keep us from noticing an emergency at all group behavior may lead us to define the situation as one that does not require action and when other people are there to share the essence of responsibility, we may feel less obligated to do something when action is required. Therefore, it will often be the case that the more people who witness his distress, the less likely it is that the victim of an emergency will get help. Thus, the stamp of the unconcerne d, depersonalized homo urbanis, blandly watching the misfortunes of others, proves inaccurate.Instead, we find a bystander to an emergency is an anguished individual in good doubt, concerned to do the right thing but compelled to make complex decisions under pressure of stress and fear. His reactions are shaped by the actions of others and all too frequently by their inaction. And we are that bystander. Caught up by the apparent indifference of others, we may pass by an emergency without helping or even realizing that help is needed. Aware of the influence of those around us, however, we can resist it. We can choose to see distress and step forward to relieve it.

No comments:

Post a Comment

Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.